翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Sizeh
・ Sizeism
・ Sizemore
・ Sizeof
・ Sizer
・ Sizer Barker
・ Sizer School
・ Sizergh Castle and Garden
・ Sizerock, Kentucky
・ Sizerville State Park
・ Sizewell
・ Sizewell Belts
・ Sizewell Hall
・ Sizewell nuclear power stations
・ Sizeze
Size–weight illusion
・ Sizhou
・ Siziano
・ Sizihwan
・ Sizihwan Station
・ Sizing
・ Sizing Europe
・ Sizing Up the Senate
・ Sizinkiler
・ Siziwang Banner
・ Sizkuh
・ Siznak
・ Sizo Maseko
・ Sizofiran
・ Sizun


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Size–weight illusion : ウィキペディア英語版
Size–weight illusion
The size–weight illusion, also known as the Charpentier illusion, is named after the French physician Augustin Charpentier〔Charpentier A. Analyse experimentale: De quelques elements de la sensation de poids (study of some aspects of weight perception ). Arch Physiol Norm Pathol 3: 122–135, 1891.〕 because he was the first to demonstrate the illusion experimentally.〔Koseleff P. Studies in the perception of heaviness. Acta Psychol 13: 242–252, 1957.〕〔Murray D J, Ellis R R, Bandomir C A, Ross H E. Charpentier (1891) on the size-weight illusion. Percept Psychophys 61: 1681-1685, 1999.〕 The illusion occurs when a person underestimates the weight of a larger object (e.g. a box) when compared to a smaller object of the same mass. Similar illusions occurs with differences in material and colour: metal containers feel lighter than wooden containers of the same size and mass,〔Seashore C E. Some psychological statistics.2. The material weight illusion. Univ Iowa Stud Psychol 2: 36-46, 1899.〕 and darker objects feel lighter than brighter objects of the same size and mass.〔De Camp J E. The influence of color on apparent weight: a preliminary study. J exp Psychol 2: 347-370, 1917.〕 These illusions can all be described as contrast with the expected weight.〔Jones LA. Perception of force and weight: theory and research. Psychol Bull 100: 29–42, 1986.〕 The expected weight or density can be measured by matching visible and hidden weights, lifted in the same manner. This gives an expected density of about 1.7 for metal canisters and 0.14 for polystyrene blocks.〔Ross, H.E. When is a weight not illusory? Q J exp Psychol 21:346-355, 1969.〕 Density expectations may assist in selecting suitable objects to throw.〔Zhu Q, Bingham G. Learning to perceive the affordance for long-distance throwing: Smart mechanism or function learning? J exp Psychol: Human Percept Perform 36: 862-875, 2010.〕
An early explanation of these illusions was that people judge the weight of an object from its appearance and then lift it with a pre-determined force. They expect a larger object to be heavier and therefore lift it with greater force: the larger object is then lifted more easily than the smaller one, causing it to be perceived as lighter.〔Müller GE, Schumann F. Über die psychologischen Grundlagen der Vergleichung gehobener Gewichte. Pflügers Arch XLV: 37–112, 1889.〕 This hypothesis was disproved by an experiment in which two objects of the same mass, same cross section, but different height were placed on observers' supported hands, and produced a passive size–weight illusion.〔Usnadze D.Über die Gewichtstauschung und ihre Analoga (of weight illusions ). Psychol Forsch 14: 366–379, 1931.〕 Recent studies have also shown that the lifting force quickly adapts to the true mass of the objects, but the size–weight illusion remains.〔Mon-Williams M, Murray A H. The size of the visual size cue used for programming manipulative forces during precision grip. Exp Brain Res 135: 405-410, 2000.〕〔Flanagan J R, Beltzner M A. Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size-weight illusion. Nature Neuroscience 3: 737-741, 2000.〕〔Brayanov J, Smith, M. Bayesian and "Anti-Bayesian" biases in sensory integration for action and perception in the size–weight illusion. J Neurophysiol 103: 1518-1531, 2010.〕 The illusion therefore cannot be explained by the manner of lifting, and must be due to some perceptual rescaling based on prior expectations. The rescaling has been described as sub-optimal (anti-Bayesian), in that the central nervous system integrates prior expectations with current proprioceptive information in a way that emphasises the unexpected information rather than taking an average of all information.〔〔Ernst M O. Perceptual learning: inverting the size-weight illusion. Curr Biol 19: R23–R25, 2009.〕
Other models describe the rescaling as partly beneficial, in that it enhances discrimination. Contrast effects are common in many perceptual modalities, and are similar to physiological adaptation. Adaptation can be explained as a change in the gain of the system, the gain being set to the appropriate level for maximum discrimination and for protection against sensory overload. Contrast effects may similarly be related to efficient neural coding.〔 If the selected range is either too high or too low, as in the size–weight illusion, there is both a contrast illusion and a loss of discrimination. It has been found that weight discrimination deteriorates if objects are lighter than their expected density,〔Ross H E, Gregory R L. Weight illusions and weight discrimination - a revised hypothesis. Q J exp Psychol 22: 318-328, 1970.〕〔Jones L F, Burgess P R. Neural gain changes subserving perceptual acuity. Somatosensory Motor Res 15: 190-199, 1998.〕 or heavier than their expected density.〔 Models of this type can account for perceptual rescaling without involving the manner of lifting.
==References==


抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Size–weight illusion」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.